Skip to main content

πŸ“° Summary (use your own words)

The Bayesian AB testing approach does mitigate the problem of peeking but it still increases the Type I error which is not the promise that the Bayesian approach promises. Conversely, because the frequentist approach is promising a Type I error rate in the form of p-value testing, it is explicitly breaking that promise if we peeked and took action on the experiments.

✍️ Notes

  • We’ve concluded thatΒ this advantage of Bayesian methods is overstated, or at least oversimplified.Β Bayesian A/B testing is not β€œimmune” to peeking and early-stopping.
  • Just like frequentist methods, peeking makes it more likely you’ll falsely stop a test. The Bayesian approach is, rather,Β more careful than the frequentist approach about what promises it makes.
  • Bayesian method is less concerned about the null hypothesis testing but rather posterior expected loss: the average amount we would lose by switching from A to B
    • Expected loss is the combination of how probably that B has a less click through rate, and if B is worse, how much worse it is on average
    • If we peeked, we would still be increasing our Type I Error
    • However, Bayesian method doesn't make the promise of controlling for the Type I Error (unlike the frequentist approach)
  • If you plotted the two curves of peeking and not-peeking against the expected loss of a perfect prior, you will find that both performance is below that line - but you would still experience an increase in Type I error
    • The goal set by the Bayesian approach was never violated
    • Maybe the problem was that the goal of controlling for Type I error is misguided
  • However the downside of the Bayesian approach do exists
    • Doesn't concern about the expected gain, so null effects might be significant